Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews
Print ISSN: 0010-7549
Published by: American Psychological Association
Publisher Location: US
DOI: 10.1037/006196
Copyright year: 1960
Copyright holder: American Psychological Association
Volume: 5, Issue: 11, Nov 1960, pages 353-355
Print pub-date: November 1960
Holistic personality
ARTHUR R. JENSEN
The reviewer identifies the authors in his review. Dr. Jensen himself
is Research Associate in the Institute of Personality Assessment and
Research at the University of California in Berkeley and also
Assistant Professor of Educational Psychology. It was Percival
Symonds at Teachers College, Columbia, whose assistant Jensen was for
three years, who interested him in the subject of personality.
Symonds and Jensen have written a book together on the development of
personality, a volume that the Columbia University Press should
publish shortly. Between his PhD and his going to Berkeley, Jensen
had two years with Eysenck in the Maudsley Laboratory in London.
While there he wrote the 1958 chapter on Personality in the Annual
Review of Psychology.
Product: 1962-01013-000. Toward Understanding Human Personalities
Robert Ward Leeper and Peter Madison New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1959. Pp. xvi + 439. $5.50.
History: PsycINFO ReleaseDate: 23-10-2006; PsycCRITIQUES ReleaseDate:
23-10-2006; PsycINFO CorrectionDate: 21-08-2017; PsycCRITIQUES
CorrectionDate: 21-08-2017
Abstract
Reviews the book, _Toward Understanding Human Personalities_ by
Robert Ward Leeper and Peter Madison (see record 1962-01013-000).
This book does not present a new theory of personality nor is it
theoretical in any systematic sense. What the authors have attempted
and have done admirably well, is to spread before the reader a vast
array of real-life phenomena that are the raw materials of
personality research. In their effort to present a panoramic view of
the domain of personality, the authors have drawn upon innumerable,
personal anecdotes, case histories, records of psychotherapy, student
autobiographies, descriptive anthropology, and works of fiction. This
book does not suggest that personality can be treated as a natural
science amenable to rigorous research. It contains however hardly any
mention of biological and hereditary aspects of personality, or of
the great amount of work that has already been done in the
measurement and assessment of personality, or of the statistical and
experimental methods that might advance our knowledge of personality.
(PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2017 APA, all rights reserved)
Keywords: personalities; research; case histories; psychotherapy;
student autobiographies; anthropology
This book is the fruit of almost a lifetime of thinking about
personality. Though Robert Leeper, Chairman of the Department of
Psychology at the University of Oregon, is best known for his work in
the fields of learning and cognition and for his critiques of Clark
Hull and Kurt Lewin, he tells us in the Preface that his major
interest has always been in personality. He began thinking about this
book as an undergraduate in the 1920s and began writing it in 1937.
He has worked on it steadily for twenty-two years. In the last five
years he had as collaborator one of his former students, Peter
Madison, a Harvard PhD in clinical psychology, now at Swarthmore
College. The book is a complete amalgam of their joint efforts.
The field of personality today is hardly a science in the sense of
being in possession of a body of verified laws and theories. It can
be called a scientific field only inasmuch as we may regard its
subject matter as a realm of natural phenomena which may be subjected
to the usual methods of science, that is to say, to systematic
description, classification, measurement, analysis, the discovery of
functional relationships, and the eventual comprehension of these
relationships or laws under a general model or theory.
Headway is made in this endeavor by scrupulous attention to parsimony
and precision in the use of language, to operational definition and,
in the early stages at least, to simplification and abstraction. All
this must proceed in close proximity to empirical realities, to
controlled observation and experimentation. The signs of progress in
our understanding of personality are becoming apparent, not so much
as yet in the form of substantive knowledge about personality as in
the development of methods for studying personality scientifically.
Progress is being made, though we are still just at the beginning of
a science of personality and still have far to go even in the
development of our tools of investigation. But this is the direction
we must follow if we are to understand personality in the sense
referred to as Wissenschaft, that is, in the objective, analytical,
nomothetic sense.
In contrast to Wissenschaft is another kind of understanding called
Verstehen, an intuitive, holistic, empathic, appreciative way of
viewing and interpreting phenomena in terms of one's own feeling
states. It is more in this tradition of Verstehen rather than
Wissenschaft that Leeper and Madison speak of "understanding human
personalities." Both the strengths and weaknesses of their work are
largely a result of this holistic, idiographic, Verstehen-type
approach.
In dedicating the book to Kohler and Tolman, the authors acknowledge
their indebtedness to the gestalt-field theory. They have written the
first introductory textbook in the personality field having a
field-theoretical orientation. (Gardner Murphy's Personality may come
to mind, but it is far more eclectic.) The authors have been
influenced also by psychologists such as Adler, Rank, Horney,
Sullivan, Carl Rogers, and George Kelly. They also owe much to Freud,
but they disapprove of the `narrowness' of orthodox psychoanalytic
theory, with its emphasis on biological drives and the `negative'
aspects of personality.
THE book does not present a new theory of personality nor is it
theoretical in any systematic sense. What the authors have attempted
and have done admirably well - some might say they have done it too
well - is to spread before the reader a vast array of real-life
phenomena that are the raw materials of personality research. Much of
the book, perhaps half of it, is taken up with colorful, rich,
realistically detailed descriptions of various human experiences. In
their effort to present a panoramic view of the domain of
personality, the authors have drawn upon innumerable, personal
anecdotes, case histories, records of psychotherapy, student
autobiographies, descriptive anthropology, and works of fiction, in
one instance quoting a passage of 10,000 words from a novel by
Lillian Smith. On the other hand, we hear nothing at all about such
`colorless' things as measurements, questionnaires, inventories, test
scores, correlations, types, traits, factors, or dimensions.
Rather than working toward a science of personality, the authors have
merely developed a manner of speaking about personality. It is a
manner that will be easy for the layman to grasp in this day when
Freud and the unconscious are household terms. The book translates
into the language of `dynamic' and gestalt psychology what are still
essentially the layman's ways of thinking about human behavior.
The idea most insistently and pervasively expressed throughout the
book is that personality is like an iceberg, with most of its mass
submerged from view. The most important part of personality is what
lies below the surface. Behavior itself is interesting only in that
it provides clues as to what is going on underneath the behavior, or
behind it, or inside the person somewhere. This underlying something,
whatever it may be, is not conceived of in physiological terms nor is
it linked in any clear or operational way to observable behavior or
to events in the environment. The underlying "mechanisms,"
"processes," "dynamic organizations," and the like are simply a
redundant manner of speaking about behavior. For example, the authors
explain that a person tires of a particular activity because of
"satiation effects," and "satiation effects" are in evidence when a
person tires of a particular activity. Obviously nothing of an
explanatory nature is achieved by the use of the term satiation
effects.
"Perceptual processes" carry the greatest burden of explanation. The
dogs in the Solomon and Wynne experiment are said to go on avoiding
the electric-shock box even when there is no longer any shock because
there is no change in their perception of the situation. When the
dogs cease jumping, it is because of a change in their perception.
The behavioristic, anxiety-reduction interpretations of Solomon and
Wynne, Dollard and Miller, and Mowrer are not hinted at. Yet the
question is not even raised concerning how the perceptual change
comes about.
The authors do not regard all individual differences in human
behavior as personality. Personality refers only to "emotionally
significant processes." It is how the person perceives and deals with
things of emotional significance to him. Personality is largely
learned (learning consists of a change in perception); it is also a
resultant of the reintegration of past experiences brought about by
the forces of the immediate psychological field.
THE critical reader is apt to become confused by the lack of
definitional clarity in the authors' manner of speaking about
personality. Often the key words outnumber the actual concepts to
which they refer - for example, processes and mechanisms, conflict
and disunity. We read that "personality processes are perceptual
processes." Also "perceptual processes may be motivational
processes." "Emotions are motives" and "emotional processes are
perceptual processes." These "processes" are never anchored in any
way to observables. The closest the authors ever come to doing so is
to state that "the development of emotional motives ... results from
the formation and growth of neural systems that the individual
originally did not possess" (p. 217). Many of the explanations by
analogy are more puzzling than clarifying. ("Perceptual processes can
be motivational processes at the same time they are perceptual
processes, just as it is true that a person is living in Pennsylvania
at the same time he is living in Philadelphia;" "The world topples
into war because it resembles a pyramid standing on its point instead
of on its base.")
Another manner of thinking about behavior that the book inculcates,
perhaps inadvertently through the loose use of language, is the
notion that we "use" habits, we "use" motives, emotions,
"reintegrative mechanisms," and so on, as if we possessed a store of
mechanisms or processes within us that we could call upon in various
circumstances.
For what audience is the book intended? This question arouses my most
serious concern. The authors have expressly addressed themselves
primarily to the psychology undergraduate taking his first course in
personality. Certainly the book will be easy and interesting for this
audience. The style is smooth and very readable. The approach is
appropriately didactic for the undergraduate; in every chapter the
authors adhere to the rule of first telling the reader what they are
going to say, then saying it, and then telling the reader what they
have said. Consequently, more advanced students may feel that the
book is longer than necessary for its essential contents.
My concern is that I greatly doubt that this book will attract into
psychology or into the area of personality those students who have a
scientific bent. Yet it is they who are the future hope of
psychology. This book does not suggest that personality can be
treated as a natural science amenable to rigorous research. Actually,
for an introductory text in a field at this stage in its development,
the text is probably not sufficiently eclectic. It contains hardly
any mention of biological and hereditary aspects of personality, or
of the great amount of work that has already been done in the
measurement and assessment of personality, or of the statistical and
experimental methods that might advance our knowledge of personality.
In this respect the exposition stands in marked contrast to the texts
of, say, Cattell and Guilford. The names of such outstanding
researchers in personality as Cattell and Eysenck are not even
mentioned, and Guilford's name appears only in the bibliography.
Indeed, the authors eschew any mention of factor-analytic or
behavioristic research in personality. Occasionally they even make
slighting remarks about experimentation and laboratory investigation
as being "colorless and neutral."
On the other hand, this is a book that the researcher in personality
may well afford to read, if only to be reminded of the richness and
complexity of his domain. Still we know that if we are to make any
real progress in a science of personality, we must be content with a
degree of simplicity that permits functional analysis, even though we
must seek it in the laboratory. However one may disapprove the
authors' depreciation of this point of view, they must nevertheless
be commended for achieving their unique purpose of presenting
personality in a richly human perspective.